Racial Microaggressions in Everyday Life
Implications for Clinical Practice
Derald Wing Sue, Christina M. Capodilupo, Gina C. Torino, Jennifer M. Bucceri,
Aisha M. B. Holder, Kevin L. Nadal, and Marta Esquilin
Teachers College, Columbia University
Racial microaggressions are brief and commonplace daily
verbal, behavioral, or environmental indignities, whether
intentional or unintentional, that communicate hostile, de-
rogatory, or negative racial slights and insults toward
people of color. Perpetrators of microaggressions are often
unaware that they engage in such communications when
they interact with racial/ethnic minorities. A taxonomy of
racial microaggressions in everyday life was created
through a review of the social psychological literature on
aversive racism, from formulations regarding the manifes-
tation and impact of everyday racism, and from reading
numerous personal narratives of counselors (both White
and those of color) on their racial/cultural awakening.
Microaggressions seem to appear in three forms: microas-
sault, microinsult, and microinvalidation. Almost all inter-
racial encounters are prone to microaggressions; this ar-
ticle uses the White counselor client of color counseling
dyad to illustrate how they impair the development of a
therapeutic alliance. Suggestions regarding education and
training and research in the helping professions are dis-
cussed.
Keywords: microaggression, microassault, microinsult, mi-
croinvalidation, attributional ambiguity
A
lthough the civil rights movement had a signifi-
cant effect on changing racial interactions in this
society, racism continues to plague the United
States (Thompson & Neville, 1999). President Clinton’s
Race Advisory Board concluded that (a) racism is one of
the most divisive forces in our society, (b) racial legacies of
the past continue to haunt current policies and practices that
create unfair disparities between minority and majority
groups, (c) racial inequities are so deeply ingrained in
American society that they are nearly invisible, and (d)
most White Americans are unaware of the advantages they
enjoy in this society and of how their attitudes and actions
unintentionally discriminate against persons of color (Ad-
visory Board to the President’s Initiative on Race, 1998).
This last conclusion is especially problematic in the mental
health professions because most graduates continue to be
White and trained primarily in Western European models
of service delivery (D. W. Sue & Sue, 2003). For that
reason, this article focuses primarily on White therapist
client of color interactions.
Because White therapists are members of the larger
society and not immune from inheriting the racial biases of
their forebears (Burkard & Knox, 2004; D. W. Sue, 2005),
they may become victims of a cultural conditioning process
that imbues within them biases and prejudices (Abelson,
Dasgupta, Park, & Banaji, 1998; Banaji, Hardin, & Roth-
man, 1993) that discriminate against clients of color. Over
the past 20 years, calls for cultural competence in the
helping professions (American Psychological Association,
2003; D. W. Sue, Arredondo, & McDavis, 1992) have
stressed the importance of two therapist characteristics
associated with effective service delivery to racial/ethnic
minority clients: (a) awareness of oneself as a racial/cul-
tural being and of the biases, stereotypes, and assumptions
that influence worldviews and (b) awareness of the world-
views of culturally diverse clients. Achieving these two
goals is blocked, however, when White clinicians fail to
understand how issues of race influence the therapy process
and how racism potentially infects the delivery of services
to clients of color (Richardson & Molinaro, 1996). Thera-
pists who are unaware of their biases and prejudices may
unintentionally create impasses for clients of color, which
may partially explain well-documented patterns of therapy
underutilization and premature termination of therapy
among such clients (Burkard & Knox, 2004; Kearney,
Draper, & Baron, 2005). In this article, we describe and
analyze how racism in the form of racial microaggressions
is particularly problematic for therapists to identify; pro-
pose a taxonomy of racial microaggressions with potential
implications for practice, education and training, and re-
search; and use the counseling/therapy process to illustrate
how racial microaggressions can impair the therapeutic
alliance. To date, no conceptual or theoretical model of
Editor’s note. Lillian Comas-Dı´az served as the action editor for this
article before Derald Wing Sue joined the American Psychologist Edito-
rial Board as an associate editor on January 1, 2007.
Authors’ note. Derald Wing Sue, Christina M. Capodilupo, Gina C.
Torino, Jennifer M. Bucceri, Aisha M. B. Holder, Kevin L. Nadal, and
Marta Esquilin, Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology,
Teachers College, Columbia University.
Aisha M. B. Holder is now at Fordham University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to De-
rald Wing Sue, Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, Box
36, Teachers College, Columbia University, 525 West 120th Street, New
York, NY 10027. E-mail: [email protected]
271May–June 2007
American Psychologist
Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0003-066X/07/$12.00
Vol. 62, No. 4, 271–286 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.4.271
racial microaggressions has been proposed to explain their
impact on the therapeutic process.
The Changing Face of Racism
In recent history, racism in North America has undergone
a transformation, especially after the post– civil rights era
when the conscious democratic belief in equality for groups
of color directly clashed with the long history of racism in
the society (Jones, 1997; Thompson & Neville, 1999). The
more subtle forms of racism have been labeled modern
racism (McConahay, 1986), symbolic racism (Sears, 1988),
and aversive racism (Dovidio, Gaertner, Kawakami, &
Hodson, 2002). All three explanations of contemporary
racism share commonalities. They emphasize that racism
(a) is more likely than ever to be disguised and covert and
(b) has evolved from the “old fashioned” form, in which
overt racial hatred and bigotry is consciously and publicly
displayed, to a more ambiguous and nebulous form that is
more difficult to identify and acknowledge.
It appears that modern and symbolic racism are most
closely associated with political conservatives, who dis-
claim personal bigotry by strong and rigid adherence to
traditional American values (individualism, self-reliance,
hard work, etc.), whereas aversive racism is more charac-
teristic of White liberals (Dovidio & Gaertner, 1996, 2000).
Aversive racists, according to these researchers, are
strongly motivated by egalitarian values as well as antimi-
nority feelings. Their egalitarian values operate on a con-
scious level, while their antiminority feelings are less con-
scious and generally covert (DeVos & Banaji, 2005). In
some respects, these three forms of racism can be ordered
along a continuum; aversive racists are the least con-
sciously negative, followed by modern and symbolic rac-
ists, who are somewhat more prejudiced, and finally by
old-fashioned biological racists (Nelson, 2006).
Although much has been written about contemporary
forms of racism, many studies in health care (Smedley &
Smedley, 2005), education (Gordon & Johnson, 2003),
employment (Hinton, 2004), mental health (Burkard &
Knox, 2004), and other social settings (Sellers & Shelton,
2003) indicate the difficulty of describing and defining
racial discrimination that occurs via “aversive racism” or
“implicit bias”; these types of racism are difficult to iden-
tify, quantify, and rectify because of their subtle, nebulous,
and unnamed nature. Without an adequate classification or
understanding of the dynamics of subtle racism, it will
remain invisible and potentially harmful to the well-being,
self-esteem, and standard of living of people of color (U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services, 2001). Ironi-
cally, it has been proposed that the daily common experi-
ences of racial aggression that characterize aversive racism
may have significantly more influence on racial anger,
frustration, and self-esteem than traditional overt forms of
racism (Solo´rzano, Ceja, & Yosso, 2000). Furthermore, the
invisible nature of acts of aversive racism prevents perpe-
trators from realizing and confronting (a) their own com-
plicity in creating psychological dilemmas for minorities
and (b) their role in creating disparities in employment,
health care, and education.
The Manifestation of Racial
Microaggressions
In reviewing the literature on subtle and contemporary
forms of racism, we have found the term racial microag-
gressions to best describe the phenomenon in its everyday
occurrence. First coined by Pierce in 1970, the term refers
to “subtle, stunning, often automatic, and non-verbal ex-
Derald Wing
Sue
Christina M.
Capodilupo
272 May–June 2007
American Psychologist
changes which are ‘put downs’” (Pierce, Carew, Pierce-
Gonzalez, & Willis, 1978, p. 66). Racial microaggressions
have also been described as “subtle insults (verbal, non-
verbal, and/or visual) directed toward people of color, often
automatically or unconsciously” (Solo´rzano et al., 2000).
Simply stated, microaggressions are brief, everyday ex-
changes that send denigrating messages to people of color
because they belong to a racial minority group. In the world
of business, the term microinequities is used to describe
the pattern of being overlooked, underrespected, and de-
valued because of one’s race or gender. Microaggressions
are often unconsciously delivered in the form of subtle
snubs or dismissive looks, gestures, and tones. These ex-
changes are so pervasive and automatic in daily conversa-
tions and interactions that they are often dismissed and
glossed over as being innocent and innocuous. Yet, as
indicated previously, microaggressions are detrimental to
persons of color because they impair performance in a
multitude of settings by sapping the psychic and spiritual
energy of recipients and by creating inequities (Franklin,
2004; D. W. Sue, 2004).
There is an urgent need to bring greater awareness and
understanding of how microaggressions operate, their nu-
merous manifestations in society, the type of impact they
have on people of color, the dynamic interaction between
perpetrator and target, and the educational strategies
needed to eliminate them. Our attempt to define and pro-
pose a taxonomy of microaggressions is grounded in sev-
eral lines of empirical and experiential evidence in the
professional literature and in personal narratives.
First, the work by psychologists on aversive racism
(Dovidio & Gaertner, 1996; Dovidio et al., 2002), studies
suggesting the widespread existence of dissociation be-
tween implicit and explicit social stereotyping (Abelson et
al., 1998; Banaji et al., 1993; DeVos & Banaji, 2005), the
attributional ambiguity of everyday racial discrimination
(Crocker & Major, 1989), the daily manifestations of rac-
ism in many arenas of life (Plant & Peruche, 2005; Sellers
& Shelton, 2003; Vanman, Saltz, Nathan, & Warren,
2004), and multiple similarities between microaggressive
incidents and items that comprise measures of race-related
stress/perceived discrimination toward Black Americans
(Brondolo et al., 2005; Klonoff & Landrine, 1999; Utsey &
Ponterotto, 1996) and Asian Americans (Liang, Li, & Kim,
2004) all seem to lend empirical support to the concept of
racial microaggressions. Second, numerous personal narra-
tives and brief life stories on race written by White psy-
chologists and psychologists of color provide experiential
evidence for the existence of racial microaggressions in
everyday life (American Counseling Association, 1999;
Conyne & Bemak, 2005; Ponterotto, Casas, Suzuki, &
Alexander, 2001). Our analysis of the life experiences of
these individuals and the research literature in social and
counseling psychology led us to several conclusions: (a)
The personal narratives were rich with examples and inci-
dents of racial microaggressions, (b) the formulation of
microaggressions was consistent with the research litera-
ture, and (c) racial microaggressions seemed to manifest
themselves in three distinct forms.
Forms of Racial Microaggressions
Racial microaggressions are brief and commonplace daily
verbal, behavioral, and environmental indignities, whether
intentional or unintentional, that communicate hostile, de-
rogatory, or negative racial slights and insults to the target
person or group. They are not limited to human encounters
alone but may also be environmental in nature, as when a
person of color is exposed to an office setting that unin-
Gina C.
Torino
Jennifer M.
Bucceri
273May–June 2007
American Psychologist
tentionally assails his or her racial identity (Gordon &
Johnson, 2003; D. W. Sue, 2003). For example, one’s racial
identity can be minimized or made insignificant through the
sheer exclusion of decorations or literature that represents
various racial groups. Three forms of microaggressions can
be identified: microassault, microinsult, and microinvalida-
tion.
Microassault
A microassault is an explicit racial derogation character-
ized primarily by a verbal or nonverbal attack meant to hurt
the intended victim through name-calling, avoidant behav-
ior, or purposeful discriminatory actions. Referring to
someone as “colored” or “Oriental,” using racial epithets,
discouraging interracial interactions, deliberately serving a
White patron before someone of color, and displaying a
swastika are examples. Microassaults are most similar to
what has been called “old fashioned” racism conducted on
an individual level. They are most likely to be conscious
and deliberate, although they are generally expressed in
limited “private” situations (micro) that allow the perpe-
trator some degree of anonymity. In other words, people
are likely to hold notions of minority inferiority privately
and will only display them publicly when they (a) lose
control or (b) feel relatively safe to engage in a microas-
sault. Because we have chosen to analyze the unintentional
and unconscious manifestations of microaggressions, mi-
croassaults are not the focus of our article. It is important to
note, however, that individuals can also vary in the degree
of conscious awareness they show in the use of the follow-
ing two forms of microaggressions.
Microinsult
A microinsult is characterized by communications that
convey rudeness and insensitivity and demean a person’s
racial heritage or identity. Microinsults represent subtle
snubs, frequently unknown to the perpetrator, but clearly
convey a hidden insulting message to the recipient of color.
When a White employer tells a prospective candidate of
color “I believe the most qualified person should get the
job, regardless of race” or when an employee of color is
asked “How did you get your job?”, the underlying mes-
sage from the perspective of the recipient may be twofold:
(a) People of color are not qualified, and (b) as a minority
group member, you must have obtained the position
through some affirmative action or quota program and not
because of ability. Such statements are not necessarily
aggressions, but context is important. Hearing these state-
ments frequently when used against affirmative action
makes the recipient likely to experience them as aggres-
sions. Microinsults can also occur nonverbally, as when a
White teacher fails to acknowledge students of color in the
classroom or when a White supervisor seems distracted
during a conversation with a Black employee by avoiding
eye contact or turning away (Hinton, 2004). In this case,
the message conveyed to persons of color is that their
contributions are unimportant.
Microinvalidation
Microinvalidations are characterized by communications
that exclude, negate, or nullify the psychological thoughts,
feelings, or experiential reality of a person of color. When
Asian Americans (born and raised in the United States) are
complimented for speaking good English or are repeatedly
asked where they were born, the effect is to negate their
U.S. American heritage and to convey that they are per-
petual foreigners. When Blacks are told that “I don’t see
color” or “We are all human beings,” the effect is to negate
their experiences as racial/cultural beings (Helms, 1992).
Aisha M. B.
Holder
Kevin L.
Nadal
274 May–June 2007
American Psychologist
When a Latino couple is given poor service at a restaurant
and shares their experience with White friends, only to be
told “Don’t be so oversensitive” or “Don’t be so petty,” the
racial experience of the couple is being nullified and its
importance is being diminished.
We have been able to identify nine categories of
microaggressions with distinct themes: alien in one’s own
land, ascription of intelligence, color blindness, criminal-
ity/assumption of criminal status, denial of individual rac-
ism, myth of meritocracy, pathologizing cultural values/
communication styles, second-class status, and environmental
invalidation. Table 1 provides samples of comments or situ-
ations that may potentially be classified as racial microaggres-
sions and their accompanying hidden assumptions and mes-
sages. Figure 1 visually presents the three large classes of
microaggressions, the classification of the themes under each
category, and their relationship to one another.
The experience of a racial microaggression has major
implications for both the perpetrator and the target person.
It creates psychological dilemmas that unless adequately
resolved lead to increased levels of racial anger, mistrust,
and loss of self-esteem for persons of color; prevent White
people from perceiving a different racial reality; and create
impediments to harmonious race-relations (Spanierman &
Heppner, 2004; Thompson & Neville, 1999).
The Invisibility and Dynamics of
Racial Microaggressions
The following real-life incident illustrates the issues of
invisibility and the disguised problematic dynamics of ra-
cial microaggressions.
I [Derald Wing Sue, the senior author, an Asian American]
recently traveled with an African American colleague on a plane
flying from New York to Boston. The plane was a small “hopper”
with a single row of seats on one side and double seats on the
other. As the plane was only sparsely populated, we were told by
the flight attendant (White) that we could sit anywhere, so we sat
at the front, across the aisle from one another. This made it easy
for us to converse and provided a larger comfortable space on a
small plane for both of us. As the attendant was about to close the
hatch, three White men in suits entered the plane, were informed
they could sit anywhere, and promptly seated themselves in front
of us. Just before take-off, the attendant proceeded to close all
overhead compartments and seemed to scan the plane with her
eyes. At that point she approached us, leaned over, interrupted our
conversation, and asked if we would mind moving to the back of
the plane. She indicated that she needed to distribute weight on
the plane evenly.
Both of us (passengers of color) had similar negative reactions.
First, balancing the weight on the plane seemed reasonable, but
why were we being singled out? After all, we had boarded first
and the three White men were the last passengers to arrive. Why
were they not being asked to move? Were we being singled out
because of our race? Was this just a random event with no racial
overtones? Were we being oversensitive and petty?
Although we complied by moving to the back of the plane, both
of us felt resentment, irritation, and anger. In light of our everyday
racial experiences, we both came to the same conclusion: The
flight attendant had treated us like second-class citizens because
of our race. But this incident did not end there. While I kept
telling myself to drop the matter, I could feel my blood pressure
rising, heart beating faster, and face flush with anger. When the
attendant walked back to make sure our seat belts were fastened,
I could not contain my anger any longer. Struggling to control
myself, I said to her in a forced calm voice: “Did you know that
you asked two passengers of color to step to the rear of the ‘bus’”?
For a few seconds she said nothing but looked at me with a
horrified expression. Then she said in a righteously indignant
tone, “Well, I have never been accused of that! How dare you? I
don’t see color! I only asked you to move to balance the plane.
Anyway, I was only trying to give you more space and greater
privacy.”
Attempts to explain my perceptions and feelings only generated
greater defensiveness from her. For every allegation I made, she
seemed to have a rational reason for her actions. Finally, she
broke off the conversation and refused to talk about the incident
any longer. Were it not for my colleague who validated my
experiential reality, I would have left that encounter wondering
whether I was correct or incorrect in my perceptions. Neverthe-
less, for the rest of the flight, I stewed over the incident and it left
a sour taste in my mouth.
The power of racial microaggressions lies in their
invisibility to the perpetrator and, oftentimes, the recipient
(D. W. Sue, 2005). Most White Americans experience
themselves as good, moral, and decent human beings who
believe in equality and democracy. Thus, they find it dif-
ficult to believe that they possess biased racial attitudes and
may engage in behaviors that are discriminatory (D. W.
Sue, 2004). Microaggressive acts can usually be explained
away by seemingly nonbiased and valid reasons. For the
recipient of a microaggression, however, there is always
the nagging question of whether it really happened
(Crocker & Major, 1989). It is difficult to identify a mi-
croaggression, especially when other explanations seem
plausible. Many people of color describe a vague feeling
Marta
Esquilin
275May–June 2007
American Psychologist
Table 1
Examples of Racial Microaggressions
Theme Microaggression Message
Alien in own land
When Asian Americans and Latino
Americans are assumed to be
foreign-born
“Where are you from?”
“Where were you born?”
“You speak good English.”
You are not American.
A person asking an Asian American to
teach them words in their native
language
You are a foreigner.
Ascription of intelligence
Assigning intelligence to a person
of color on the basis of their race
“You are a credit to your race.” People of color are generally not as
intelligent as Whites.
“You are so articulate.” It is unusual for someone of your race
to be intelligent.
Asking an Asian person to help with a
math or science problem
All Asians are intelligent and good in
math/sciences.
Color blindness
Statements that indicate that a
White person does not want to
acknowledge race
“When I look at you, I don’t see color.” Denying a person of color’s racial/
ethnic experiences.
“America is a melting pot.” Assimilate/acculturate to the
dominant culture.
“There is only one race, the human race.” Denying the individual as a racial/
cultural being.
Criminality/assumption of criminal
status
A person of color is presumed to be
dangerous, criminal, or deviant
on the basis of their race
A White man or woman clutching their
purse or checking their wallet as a
Black or Latino approaches or passes
You are a criminal.
A store owner following a customer of
color around the store
You are going to steal/ You are
poor/ You do not belong.
A White person waits to ride the next
elevator when a person of color is on it
You are dangerous.
Denial of individual racism
A statement made when Whites
deny their racial biases
“I’m not racist. I have several Black
friends.”
I am immune to racism because I
have friends of color.
“As a woman, I know what you go
through as a racial minority.”
Your racial oppression is no different
than my gender oppression. I can’t
be a racist. I’m like you.
Myth of meritocracy
Statements which assert that race
does not play a role in life
successes
“I believe the most qualified person should
get the job.”
People of color are given extra unfair
benefits because of their race.
“Everyone can succeed in this society, if
they work hard enough.”
People of color are lazy and/or
incompetent and need to work
harder.
Pathologizing cultural values/
communication styles
The notion that the values and
communication styles of the
dominant/White culture are ideal
Asking a Black person: “Why do you
have to be so loud/animated? Just calm
down.”
Assimilate to dominant culture.
To an Asian or Latino person: “Why are
you so quiet? We want to know what
you think. Be more verbal.” “Speak up
more.”
Dismissing an individual who brings up
race/culture in work/school setting
Leave your cultural baggage outside.
Second-class citizen
Occurs when a White person is
given preferential treatment as a
consumer over a person of color
Person of color mistaken for a service
worker
People of color are servants to
Whites. They couldn’t possibly
occupy high-status positions.
Having a taxi cab pass a person of color
and pick up a White passenger
You are likely to cause trouble and/
or travel to a dangerous
neighborhood.
276 May–June 2007
American Psychologist
that they have been attacked, that they have been disre-
spected, or that something is not right (Franklin, 2004; Reid
& Radhakrishnan, 2003). In some respects, people of color
may find an overt and obvious racist act easier to handle
than microaggressions that seem vague or disguised (So-
lo´rzano et al., 2000). The above incident reveals how
microaggressions operate to create psychological dilemmas
for both the White perpetrator and the person of color. Four
such dilemmas are particularly noteworthy for everyone to
understand.
Dilemma 1: Clash of Racial Realities
The question we pose is this: Did the flight attendant
engage in a microaggression or did the senior author and
his colleague simply misinterpret the action? Studies indi-
cate that the racial perceptions of people of color differ
markedly from those of Whites (Jones, 1997; Harris Poll
commissioned by the National Conference of Christians
and Jews, 1992). In most cases, White Americans tend to
believe that minorities are doing better in life, that discrim-
ination is on the decline, that racism is no longer a signif-
icant factor in the lives of people of color, and that equality
has been achieved. More important, the majority of Whites
do not view themselves as racist or capable of racist be-
havior.
Minorities, on the other hand, perceive Whites as (a)
racially insensitive, (b) unwilling to share their position and
wealth, (c) believing they are superior, (d) needing to
control everything, and (e) treating them poorly because of
their race. People of color believe these attributes are
reenacted everyday in their interpersonal interactions with
Whites, oftentimes in the form of microaggressions (Solo´r-
zano et al., 2000). For example, it was found that 96% of
African Americans reported experiencing racial discrimi-
nation in a one-year period (Klonoff & Landrine, 1999),
and many incidents involved being mistaken for a service
worker, being ignored, given poor service, treated rudely,
or experiencing strangers acting fearful or intimidated
when around them (Sellers & Shelton, 2003).
Dilemma 2: The Invisibility of Unintentional
Expressions of Bias
The interaction between the senior author and the flight
attendant convinced him that she was sincere in her belief
that she had acted in good faith without racial bias. Her
actions and their meaning were invisible to her. It was clear
that she was stunned that anyone would accuse her of such
despicable actions. After all, in her mind, she acted with
only the best of intentions: to distribute the weight evenly
on the plane for safety reasons and to give two passengers
greater privacy and space. She felt betrayed that her good
intentions were being questioned. Yet considerable empir-
ical evidence exists showing that racial microaggressions
become automatic because of cultural conditioning and that
they may become connected neurologically with the pro-
cessing of emotions that surround prejudice (Abelson et al.,
1998). Several investigators have found, for example, that
law enforcement officers in laboratory experiments will fire
their guns more often at Black criminal suspects than White
ones (Plant & Peruche, 2005), and Afrocentric features
tend to result in longer prison terms (Blair, Judd, & Chap-
leau, 2004). In all cases, these law enforcement officials
had no conscious awareness that they responded differently
on the basis of race.
Herein lies a major dilemma. How does one prove that
a microaggression has occurred? What makes our belief
that the flight attendant acted in a biased manner any more
plausible than her conscious belief that it was generated for
another reason? If she did act out of hidden and uncon-
scious bias, how do we make her aware of it? Social
psychological research tends to confirm the existence of
unconscious racial biases in well-intentioned Whites, that
Table 1 (continued)
Theme Microaggression Message
Second-class citizen (continued)
Occurs when a White person is
given preferential treatment as a
consumer over a person of color
(continued)
Being ignored at a store counter as
attention is given to the White customer
behind you
Whites are more valued customers
than people of color.
“You people...” Youdon’t belong. You are a lesser
being.
Environmental microaggressions
Macro-level microaggressions,
which are more apparent on
systemic and environmental levels
A college or university with buildings that
are all named after White heterosexual
upper class males
You don’t belong/You won’t succeed
here. There is only so far you can
go.
Television shows and movies that feature
predominantly White people, without
representation of people of color
You are an outsider/You don’t exist.
Overcrowding of public schools in
communities of color
People of color don’t/shouldn’t value
education.
Overabundance of liquor stores in
communities of color
People of color are deviant.
277May–June 2007
American Psychologist
nearly everyone born and raised in the United States inher-
its the racial biases of the society, and that the most
accurate assessment about whether racist acts have oc-
curred in a particular situation is most likely to be made by
those most disempowered rather than by those who enjoy
the privileges of power (Jones, 1997; Keltner & Robinson,
1996). According to these findings, microaggressions (a)
tend to be subtle, indirect, and unintentional, (b) are most
likely to emerge not when a behavior would look prejudi-
cial, but when other rationales can be offered for prejudi-
cial behavior, and (c) occur when Whites pretend not to
notice differences, thereby justifying that “color” was not
involved in the actions taken. Color blindness is a major
form of microinvalidation because it denies the racial and
experiential reality of people of color and provides an
excuse to White people to claim that they are not preju-
diced (Helms, 1992; Neville, Lilly, Duran, Lee, & Browne,
2000). The flight attendant, for example, did not realize that
her “not seeing color” invalidated both passengers’ racial
identity and experiential reality.
Dilemma 3: Perceived Minimal Harm of
Racial Microaggressions
In most cases, when individuals are confronted with their
microaggressive acts (as in the case of the flight attendant),
the perpetrator usually believes that the victim has overre-
acted and is being overly sensitive and/or petty. After all,
even if it was an innocent racial blunder, microaggressions
are believed to have minimal negative impact. People of
color are told not to overreact and to simply “let it go.”
Usually, Whites consider microaggressive incidents to be
Figure 1
Categories of and Relationships Among Racial Microaggressions
Ascription of Intelligence
Alien in Own Land
Assigning a degree of intelligence to a person of
color based on their race.
Second Class Citizen
Treated as a lesser person or group.
Pathologizing cultural
values/communication styles
Notion that the values and communication styles
of people of color are abnormal.
Assumption of Criminal status
Presumed to be a criminal, dangerous, or deviant
based on race.
Belief that visible racial/ethnic minority
citizens are foreigners.
Color Blindness
Denial or pretense that a White person does not
see color or race.
Myth of Meritocracy
Statements which assert that race plays a minor
role
in life success.
Denial of Individual Racism
Denial of personal racism or one’s role in its
perpetuation.
Microinsult
(Often Unconscious)
Behavioral/verbal remarks or comments that
convey rudeness, insensitivity and demean a
person’s racial heritage or identity.
Microassault
(Often Conscious)
Explicit racial derogations characterized
primarily by a violent verbal or
nonverbal attack meant to hurt the
intended victim through name-calling,
avoidant behavior or purposeful
discriminatory actions.
Environmental
Microaggressions
(Macro-level)
Racial assaults, insults and
invalidations which are
manifested on systemic and
environmental levels.
Microinvalidation
(Often Unconscious)
Verbal comments or behaviors that
exclude, negate, or nullify the
psychological thoughts, feelings, or
experiential reality of a person of
color.
Racial Microaggressions
Commonplace verbal or behavioral indignities, whether intentional or unintentional,
which communicate hostile, derogatory, or negative racial slights and insults.
278 May–June 2007
American Psychologist
minor, and people of color are encouraged (oftentimes by
people of color as well) to not waste time or effort on them.
It is clear that old-fashioned racism unfairly disadvan-
tages people of color and that it contributes to stress,
depression, shame, and anger in its victims (Jones, 1997).
But evidence also supports the detrimental impact of more
subtle forms of racism (Chakraborty & McKenzie, 2002;
Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999). For example,
in a survey of studies examining racism and mental health,
researchers found a positive association between happiness
and life satisfaction, self-esteem, mastery of control, hy-
pertension, and discrimination (Williams, Neighbors, &
Jackson, 2003). Many of the types of everyday racism
identified by Williams and colleagues (Williams & Collins,
1995; Williams, Lavizzo-Mourey, & Warren, 1994) pro-
vide strong support for the idea that racial microaggres-
sions are not minimally harmful. One study specifically
examined microaggressions in the experiences of African
Americans and found that the cumulative effects can be
quite devastating (Solo´rzano et al., 2000). The researchers
reported that experience with microaggressions resulted in
a negative racial climate and emotions of self-doubt, frus-
tration, and isolation on the part of victims. As indicated in
the incident above, the senior author experienced consid-
erable emotional turmoil that lasted for the entire flight.
When one considers that people of color are exposed con-
tinually to microaggressions and that their effects are cu-
mulative, it becomes easier to understand the psychological
toll they may take on recipients’ well-being.
We submit that covert racism in the form of microag-
gressions also has a dramatic and detrimental impact on
people of color. Although microaggressions may be seem-
ingly innocuous and insignificant, their effects can be quite
dramatic (Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002). D. W. Sue
believes that “this contemporary form of racism is many
times over more problematic, damaging, and injurious to
persons of color than overt racist acts” (D. W. Sue, 2003,
p. 48). It has been noted that the cumulative effects of racial
microaggressions may theoretically result in “diminished
mortality, augmented morbidity and flattened confidence”
(Pierce, 1995, p. 281). It is important to study and acknowl-
edge this form of racism in society because without docu-
mentation and analysis to better understand microaggres-
sions, the threats that they pose and the assaults that they
justify can be easily ignored or downplayed (Solo´rzano et
al., 2000). D. W. Sue (2005) has referred to this phenom-
enon as “a conspiracy of silence.”
Dilemma 4: The Catch-22 of Responding to
Microaggressions
When a microaggression occurs, the victim is usually
placed in a catch-22. The immediate reaction might be a
series of questions: Did what I think happened, really
happen? Was this a deliberate act or an unintentional
slight? How should I respond? Sit and stew on it or con-
front the person? If I bring the topic up, how do I prove it?
Is it really worth the effort? Should I just drop the matter?
These questions in one form or another have been a com-
mon, if not a universal, reaction of persons of color who
experience an attributional ambiguity (Crocker & Major,
1989).
First, the person must determine whether a microag-
gression has occurred. In that respect, people of color rely
heavily on experiential reality that is contextual in nature
and involves life experiences from a variety of situations.
When the flight attendant asked the senior author and his
colleague to move, it was not the first time that similar
requests and situations had occurred for both. In their
experience, these incidents were nonrandom events (Rid-
ley, 2005), and their perception was that the only similarity
“connecting the dots” to each and every one of these
incidents was the color of their skin. In other words, the
situation on the plane was only one of many similar inci-
dents with identical outcomes. Yet the flight attendant and
most White Americans do not share these multiple experi-
ences, and they evaluate their own behaviors in the moment
through a singular event (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2000). Thus,
they fail to see a pattern of bias, are defended by a belief in
their own morality, and can in good conscience deny that
they discriminated (D. W. Sue, 2005).
Second, how one reacts to a microaggression may
have differential effects, not only on the perpetrator but on
the person of color as well. Deciding to do nothing by
sitting on one’s anger is one response that occurs frequently
in people of color. This response occurs because persons of
color may be (a) unable to determine whether a microag-
gression has occurred, (b) at a loss for how to respond, (c)
fearful of the consequences, (d) rationalizing that “it won’t
do any good anyway,” or (e) engaging in self-deception
through denial (“It didn’t happen.”). Although these expla-
nations for nonresponse may hold some validity for the
person of color, we submit that not doing anything has the
potential to result in psychological harm. It may mean a
denial of one’s experiential reality, dealing with a loss of
integrity, or experiencing pent-up anger and frustration
likely to take psychological and physical tolls.
Third, responding with anger and striking back (per-
haps a normal and healthy reaction) is likely to engender
negative consequences for persons of color as well. They
are likely to be accused of being racially oversensitive or
paranoid or told that their emotional outbursts confirm
stereotypes about minorities. In the case of Black males, for
example, protesting may lend credence to the belief that
they are hostile, angry, impulsive, and prone to violence
(Jones, 1997). In this case, the person of color might feel
better after venting, but the outcome results in greater
hostility by Whites toward minorities. Further, while the
person of color may feel better in the immediate moment
by relieving pent-up emotions, the reality is that the general
situation has not been changed. In essence, the catch-22
means you are “damned if you do, and damned if you
don’t.” What is lacking is research that points to adaptive
ways of handling microaggressions by people of color and
suggestions of how to increase the awareness and sensitiv-
ity of Whites to microaggressions so that they accept re-
sponsibility for their behaviors and for changing them
(Solo´rzano et al., 2000).
279May–June 2007
American Psychologist
Racial Microaggressions as a Barrier
to Clinical Practice
In a broad sense, counseling and psychotherapy can be
characterized as the formation of a deeply personal rela-
tionship between a helping professional and a client that
involves appropriate and accurate interpersonal interac-
tions and communications. For effective therapy to occur,
some form of positive coalition must develop between the
parties involved (D. W. Sue & Sue, 2003). Many have
referred to this as the “working relationship,” the “thera-
peutic alliance,” or the “establishment of rapport” (D. W.
Sue & Sue, 2003). A strong therapeutic relationship is often
enhanced when clients perceive therapists as credible
(trustworthy and expert) and themselves as understood and
positively regarded by their therapists (Strong & Schmidt,
1970). Helping professionals are trained to listen, to show
empathic concern, to be objective, to value the client’s
integrity, to communicate understanding, and to use their
professional knowledge and skills to aid clients to solve
problems (Grencavage & Norcross, 1990).
As a therapeutic team, therapist and client are better
prepared to venture into problematic areas that the client
might hesitate to face alone. Research suggests that the
therapeutic alliance is one of the major common factors of
any helping relationship and is correlated with successful
outcome (Lui & Pope-Davis, 2005; Martin, Garske, &
Davis, 2000). More important, however, are findings that a
client’s perception of an accepting and positive relationship
is a better predictor of successful outcome than is a similar
perception by the counselor (Horvath & Symonds, 1991).
Thus, when clients do not perceive their therapists as
trustworthy and when they feel misunderstood and under-
valued, therapeutic success is less likely to occur. Often-
times, the telltale signs of a failed therapeutic relationship
may result in clients being less likely to self-disclose,
terminating prematurely, or failing to return for scheduled
visits (Burkard & Knox, 2004; Kearney, Draper, & Baron,
2005).
Although the task of establishing an effective thera-
peutic relationship applies to the entire helping spectrum,
working with clients who differ from the therapist in race,
ethnicity, culture, and sexual orientation poses special chal-
lenges. White therapists who are products of their cultural
conditioning may be prone to engage in racial microag-
gressions (Locke & Kiselica, 1999). Thus, the therapeutic
alliance is likely to be weakened or terminated when clients
of color perceive White therapists as biased, prejudiced, or
unlikely to understand them as racial/cultural beings. That
racism can potentially infect the therapeutic process when
working with clients of color has been a common concern
voiced by the President’s Commission on Mental Health
(1978) and the Surgeon General’s Report on Mental
Health: Culture, Race and Ethnicity (U.S. Department of
Health and Human Services, 2001). It has been postulated
that therapist bias might partially account for the low
utilization of mental health services and premature termi-
nation of therapy sessions by African American, Native
American, Asian American, and Latino/Hispanic American
clients (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services,
2001).
Yet research also reveals that most people in our
nation believe in democracy, fairness, and strong human-
istic values that condemn racism and the inequities that it
engenders (Dovidio et al., 2002). Such a statement is ar-
guably truer for mental health professionals, whose goals
are to help rather than hinder or hurt clients of color. Both
the American Psychological Association and the American
Counseling Association have attempted to confront the
biases of the profession by passing multicultural guidelines
or standards that denounce prejudice and discrimination in
the delivery of mental health services to clients of color
(American Psychological Association, 2003; D. W. Sue et
al., 1992). Like most people in society, counselors and
therapists experience themselves as fair and decent indi-
viduals who would never consciously and deliberately en-
gage in racist acts toward clients of color. Sadly, it is often
pointed out that when clinician and client differ from one
another along racial lines, however, the relationship may
serve as a microcosm for the troubled race relations in the
United States. While many would like to believe that
racism is no longer a major problem and that the good
intentions of the helping profession have built safeguards
against prejudice and discrimination, the reality is that they
continue to be manifested through the therapeutic process
(Utsey, Gernat, & Hammar, 2005). This is not to suggest,
however, that positive changes in race relations have not
occurred. Yet, as in many other interactions, microaggres-
sions are equally likely to occur in therapeutic transactions
(Ridley, 2005).
The Manifestation of Racial
Microaggressions in
Counseling/Therapy
Microaggressions become meaningful in the context of
clinical practice, as relational dynamics and the human
condition are central aspects of this field. The often unin-
tentional and unconscious nature of microaggressions (Di-
lemma 2: Invisibility) poses the biggest challenge to the
majority of White mental health professionals, who believe
that they are just, unbiased, and nonracist. Further, mental
health professionals are in a position of power, which
renders them less likely to accurately assess (Dilemma 1:
Conflict of Racial Realities) whether racist acts have oc-
curred in their sessions. Thus, the harm they perpetrate
against their clients of color is either unknown or mini-
mized (Dilemma 3: Minimal Harm). Microaggressions not
only oppress and harm, but they place clients of color in the
unenviable position of a catch-22 (Dilemma 4).
In clinical practice, microaggressions are likely to go
unrecognized by White clinicians who are unintentionally
and unconsciously expressing bias. As a result, therapists
must make a concerted effort to identify and monitor mi-
croaggressions within the therapeutic context. This process
is reminiscent of the importance of becoming aware of
potential transference and countertransference issues be-
tween therapist and client and how they may unintention-
280 May–June 2007
American Psychologist
ally interfere with effective therapy (Woodhouse, Schlosser,
Crook, Ligiero, & Gelso, 2003). The inherent power dy-
namic in the therapeutic relationship further complicates
this issue, as therapists are in a position of power to make
diagnoses and influence the course of treatment. The power
dynamic between therapist and client also effects the
catch-22 of responding to microaggressions because clients
may be less likely to confront their therapists and more
likely to question their own perceptions in the event of a
microaggression.
Table 2 provides a few examples of microaggressions
in counseling practice under each of the nine categories
identified earlier. Under Color Blindness, for example, a
client of color stresses the importance of racial experiences
only to have the therapist reply, “We are all unique. We are
all individuals.” or “We are all human beings or the same
under the skin.” These colorblind statements, which were
intended to be supportive, to be sympathetic, and to convey
an ability to understand, may leave the client feeling mis-
understood, negated, invalidated, and unimportant (espe-
cially if racial identity is important to the client). Moreover
these statements presume that the therapist is capable of
not seeing race and impose a definition of racial reality on
the client (Neville et al., 2000).
Under Denial of Individual Racism, a common re-
sponse by Whites to people of color is that they can
understand and relate to experiences of racism. In Table 2,
under this category, we provide the following anecdote: A
client of color expresses hesitancy in discussing racial
issues with his White female therapist. She replies, “I
understand. As a woman, I face discrimination too.” The
message is that the therapist believes her gender oppression
is no different from the client’s experiences of racial/ethnic
oppression. This response is problematic because such at-
tempts by the therapist to explain how he or she can
understand a person of color’s experience with racism may
be perceived by the client as an attempt to minimize the
importance of his or her racial identity, to avoid acknowl-
edging the therapist’s racial biases, or to communicate a
discomfort with discussing racial issues. Furthermore, the
therapist excuses himself or herself from any blame or fault
in perpetuating racism and the power of racism. This fail-
ure to acknowledge the significance of racism within and
outside of the therapy session contributes to the breakdown
of the alliance between therapist and client. A therapist’s
willingness to discuss racial matters is of central impor-
tance in creating a therapeutic alliance with clients of color
(Cardemil & Battle, 2003).
Under the category “Alien in Own Land,” many Asian
Americans and Latino/Hispanic Americans report that they
are commonly seen as perpetual foreigners. For example, a
female Asian American client arrives for her first therapy
session. Her therapist asks her where she is from, and when
told “Philadelphia,” the therapist further probes by asking
where she was born. In this case, the therapist has assumed
that the Asian American client is not from the United States
and has imposed through the use of the second question the
idea that she must be a foreigner. Immediately, a barrier is
created in the helping relationship because the client feels
invalidated by the therapist (she is perceived as a foreigner,
not a U.S. citizen). Unfortunately, the Asian American
client is unlikely to question her therapist or point out the
bias because of the power dynamic, which causes her to
harbor resentment and ill feelings toward the therapist.
We contend that clients of color are at increased risk
of not continuing in the counseling/therapy session when
such microaggressions occur. Worse yet, they will not
receive the help they need and may leave the session
feeling worse than when they first sought counseling. Be-
cause it is unlikely that clinicians intentionally create hos-
tile and unwelcoming environments for their ethnic minor-
ity clients, it can be assumed that these biases are being
expressed through microaggressions. Therapists can con-
vey their bias to their ethnic minority clients in myriad
ways, such as by minimizing symptoms for Asian Ameri-
cans on the basis of a false belief in the “model” minority
(D. W. Sue & Sue, 2003) or by placing greater emphasis on
symptoms such as paranoid delusions and substance abuse
in Native Americans and Africans Americans, who are
believed to suffer from these afflictions (U.S. Department
of Health and Human Services, 2001).
Last, White counselors and therapists can impose and
value their own cultural worldview while devaluing and
pathologizing the cultural values of their ethnic minority
clients. Previous research has shown that pathologizing
clients’ cultural values has been a major determinant of
clients of color discontinuing psychotherapy (S. Sue, Fu-
jino, Hu, & Takeuchi, 1991). Many clients of color may
feel misunderstood by their therapists because of a lack of
cultural understanding. Asian American or Latino Ameri-
can clients who enter therapy to discuss family issues such
as feeling obligated, stressed, or overwhelmed with excess
family responsibilities may be encouraged by therapists to
speak out against their families or to make decisions re-
gardless of family support or expectations. Therapists may
be unaware that they may be directly invalidating cultural
respect for authority and imposing an individualistic view
over a collectivist one.
Future Directions in the
Understanding of Racial
Microaggressions
With respect to racism, D. W. Sue (2004, p. 762) has stated
that the greatest challenge society and the mental health
professions face is “making the ‘invisible’ visible.” That
can only be accomplished when people are willing to
openly and honestly engage in a dialogue about race and
racism. In that respect, the education and training of mental
health professionals must incorporate issues of race and
culture. One would ordinarily expect that mental health
professionals would be more willing than most to dialogue
on this topic, but studies suggest that White clinicians
receive minimal or no practicum or supervision experi-
ences that address race and are uncomfortable broaching
the topic (Knox, Burkard, Johnson, Suzuki, & Ponterotto,
2003). Many White trainees in therapy dyads experience
anxiety in the form of poor articulation, faltering and/or
281May–June 2007
American Psychologist
Table 2
Examples of Racial Microaggressions in Therapeutic Practice
Theme Microaggression Message
Alien in own land
When Asian Americans and
Latino Americans are
assumed to be foreign-born
A White client does not want to work with an Asian
American therapist because “she will not
understand my problem.”
You are not American.
A White therapist tells an American-born Latino
client that he/she should seek a Spanish-speaking
therapist.
Ascription of intelligence
Assigning a degree of
intelligence to a person of
color on the basis of their
race
A school counselor reacts with surprise when an
Asian American student had trouble on the math
portion of a standardized test.
All Asians are smart and good at
math.
A career counselor asking a Black or Latino student,
“Do you think you’re ready for college?”
It is unusual for people of color to
succeed.
Color blindness
Statements which indicate that
a White person does not
want to acknowledge race
A therapist says “I think you are being too
paranoid. We should emphasize similarities, not
people’s differences” when a client of color
attempts to discuss her feelings about being the
only person of color at her job and feeling
alienated and dismissed by her co-workers.
Race and culture are not
important variables that affect
people’s lives.
A client of color expresses concern in discussing
racial issues with her therapist. Her therapist
replies with, “When I see you, I don’t see color.”
Your racial experiences are not
valid.
Criminality/assumption of
criminal status
A person of color is presumed
to be dangerous, criminal,
or deviant on the basis of
their race
When a Black client shares that she was accused of
stealing from work, the therapist encourages the
client to explore how she might have contributed
to her employer’s mistrust of her.
You are a criminal.
A therapist takes great care to ask all substance
abuse questions in an intake with a Native
American client, and is suspicious of the client’s
nonexistent history with substances.
You are deviant.
Denial of individual racism
A statement made when
Whites renounce their
racial biases
A client of color asks his or her therapist about how
race affects their working relationship. The
therapist replies, “Race does not affect the way I
treat you.”
Your racial/ethnic experience is
not important.
A client of color expresses hesitancy in discussing
racial issues with his White female therapist. She
replies “I understand. As a woman, I face
discrimination also.”
Your racial oppression is no
different than my gender
oppression.
Myth of meritocracy
Statements which assert that
race does not play a role in
succeeding in career
advancement or education.
A school counselor tells a Black student that “if you
work hard, you can succeed like everyone else.”
A career counselor is working with a client of color
who is concerned about not being promoted at
work despite being qualified. The counselor
suggests, “Maybe if you work harder you can
succeed like your peers.”
People of color are lazy and/or
incompetent and need to work
harder. If you don’t succeed,
you have only yourself to
blame (blaming the victim).
Pathologizing cultural values/
communication styles
The notion that the values and
communication styles of the
dominant/White culture are
ideal
A Black client is loud, emotional, and
confrontational in a counseling session. The
therapist diagnoses her with borderline
personality disorder.
Assimilate to dominant culture.
A client of Asian or Native American descent has
trouble maintaining eye contact with his therapist.
The therapist diagnoses him with a social anxiety
disorder.
Advising a client, “Do you really think your problem
stems from racism?”
Leave your cultural baggage
outside.
282 May–June 2007
American Psychologist
trembling voices, and mispronunciation of words when
directly engaged in discussions about race (Utsey et al.,
2005). It is interesting that such nonverbal behaviors also
serve as a form of racial microaggression. When helping
professionals have difficulty addressing race issues, they
cut off an avenue for clients of color to explore matters of
bias, discrimination, and prejudice.
Education and Training and Racial
Microaggressions
It is clear that mental health training programs must support
trainees in overcoming their fears and their resistance to
talking about race by fostering safe and productive learning
environments (Sanchez-Hucles & Jones, 2005). It is im-
portant that training programs be structured and facilitated
in a manner that promotes inquiry and allows trainees to
experience discomfort and vulnerability (Young & Davis-
Russell, 2002). Trainees need to be challenged to explore
their own racial identities and their feelings about other
racial groups. The prerequisite for cultural competence has
always been racial self-awareness. This is equally true for
understanding how microaggressions, especially those of
the therapist, influence the therapeutic process. This level
of self-awareness brings to the surface possible prejudices
and biases that inform racial microaggressions. A first step
for therapists who want to integrate an understanding of
racism’s mental health effects into the conceptualization of
psychological functioning is to undergo a process of learn-
ing and critical self-examination of racism and its impact
on one’s life and the lives of others (Thompson & Neville,
1999). For White clinicians, it means addressing the ques-
tion “What does it mean to be White?” and being fully
cognizant of their own White racial identity development
and how it may intrude on people of color (Helms, 1992,
1995). In addition, it has been suggested that articulating a
personal theory of reality and of therapeutic change in the
context of an environment of racism is one way to begin
integrating knowledge of racism with the practice of psy-
chotherapy (Thompson & Neville, 1999). Education and
training must aid White clinicians to achieve the following:
(a) increase their ability to identify racial microaggressions
in general and in themselves in particular; (b) understand
how racial microaggressions, including their own, detri-
mentally impact clients of color; and (c) accept responsi-
bility for taking corrective actions to overcome racial bi-
ases.
Research on Racial Microaggressions
A major obstacle to understanding racial microaggressions
is that research is in a nascent state. Researchers continue
to omit subtle racism and microaggressions from their
research agendas, and this absence conveys the notion that
covert forms of racism are not as valid or as important as
racist events that can be quantified and “proven.” In fact,
omitting microaggressions from studies on racism on the
basis of a belief that they are less harmful encourages the
profession to “look the other way.” Moreover, the fact that
psychological research has continued to inadequately ad-
dress race and ethnicity (Delgado-Romero, Rowland, &
Galvin, 2005) is in itself a microaggression. Pursuing a line
of research examining how cross-racial dyadic composi-
tions impact the process and outcome of counselor/client
interactions would be a tremendous contribution to the field
of counseling and clinical psychology. Helms and Cook
(1999) noted that racial consciousness is a critical consid-
eration in determining White therapists’ ability to operate
effectively in cross-racial dyads.
For mental health purposes, it would be useful to
explore the coping mechanisms used by people of color to
stave off the negative effects of microaggressions. The fact
that people of color have had to face daily microaggres-
sions and have continued to maintain their dignity in the
face of such hostility is a testament to their resiliency
(D. W. Sue, 2003). What coping strategies have been found
to serve them well? A greater understanding of responses to
microaggressions, both in the long term and the short term,
and of the coping strategies employed would be beneficial
in arming children of color for the life they will face. Such
research is necessary because without documentation and
analysis to help better understand microaggressions, the
threats that they pose and the assaults that they justify can
be easily ignored or downplayed (Solo´rzano et al., 2000).
Table 2 (continued)
Theme Microaggression Message
Second-class citizen
Occurs when a White person
is given preferential
treatment as a consumer
over a person of color
A counselor limits the amount of long-term
therapy to provide at a college
counseling center; she chooses all
White clients over clients of color.
Whites are more valued than people of
color.
Clients of color are not welcomed or
acknowledged by receptionists.
White clients are more valued than clients
of color.
Environmental microaggressions
Macro-level microaggressions,
which are more apparent
on a systemic level
A waiting room office has pictures of
American presidents.
You don’t belong/Only white people can
succeed.
Every counselor at a mental health clinic is
White.
You are an outsider/You don’t exist.
283May–June 2007
American Psychologist
Studying the long-term impact that microaggressions have
on mental health functioning, self-esteem, self-concept,
and racial identity development appears crucial to docu-
menting the harm microaggressions inflict on people of
color. The taxonomy of microaggressions proposed here
may make it easier to explore other social psychological
questions as well.
First, it is highly probable that microaggressions vary
in their severity and impact. As indicated, a microassault
does not evoke a guessing game because the intent of the
perpetrator is clear. However, the racist intent of microin-
sults and microinvalidations is less clear and presents dif-
ferent dilemmas for people of color. Some questions to
ponder include the following: (a) Are the three forms of
racial microaggressions equal in impact? Are some themes
and their hidden messages more problematic than others?
Although all expressions may take a psychological toll,
some are obviously experienced as more harmful and se-
vere than others. (b) Is there a relationship between forms
of racial microaggressions and racial identity development?
Recent research and formulations on White racial identity
development and the psychosocial costs of racism to
Whites (Helms, 1995; Spanierman, Armstrong, Poteat, &
Beer, 2006) imply that forms of racial microaggressions
may be associated with certain statuses or trait clusters. (c)
Finally, is it possible that different racial/ethnic groups are
more likely to encounter certain forms of racial microag-
gressions than others? A preliminary study suggests that
Asian Americans are prone to be victims of microinvali-
dations with themes that revolve around “alien in one’s
own land” (D. W. Sue, Bucceri, Lin, Nadal, & Torino,
2007) rather than microinsults with themes of “criminal-
ity.” Is it possible that Blacks are more likely to be sub-
jected to the latter than to the former? What about Latinos
and American Indians?
Second, the challenge in conducting research aimed at
understanding microaggressions involves measurement.
Adequate assessment tools need to be created to effectively
explore the new and burgeoning field of microaggression
research. Although there are several promising race-related
stress and discrimination measures, such as the Perceived
Ethnic Discrimination Questionnaire (PEDQ; Brondolo
et al., 2005), the Color-Blind Racial Attitude Scale
(COBRAS; Neville et al., 2000), the Index of Race Related
Stress (IRRS; Utsey & Ponterotto, 1996), and the Schedule
of Racist Events (SRE; Klonoff & Landrine, 1999), none of
them is directly aimed at distinguishing between categories
of racial microaggressions or their intentional or uninten-
tional nature. The PEDQ uses four subscales that broadly
measure stigmatization, harassment, workplace discrimina-
tion, and social exclusion; the COBRAS is specific to a
person’s minimization of race and racism; the IRRS uses
Jones’s (1997) framework to measure individual, institu-
tional, and societal racism; and the SRE is aimed at mea-
suring frequency of racist incidents. All contain examples
of racial microaggressions that support our taxonomy, but
none makes conceptual distinctions that allow for categor-
ical measurements of this phenomenon. It seems impera-
tive that specific instruments be developed to aid in under-
standing the causes, consequences, manifestations, and
elimination of racial microaggressions.
Conclusion
Nearly all interracial encounters are prone to the manifes-
tation of racial microaggressions. We have chosen mainly
to address the therapeutic relationship, but racial microag-
gressions are potentially present whenever human interac-
tions involve participants who differ in race and culture
(teaching, supervising, training, administering, evaluating,
etc.). We have purposely chosen to concentrate on racial
microaggressions, but it is important to acknowledge other
types of microaggressions as well. Gender, sexual orienta-
tion, and disability microaggressions may have equally
powerful and potentially detrimental effects on women,
gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgender individuals, and
disability groups. Further, racial microaggressions are not
limited to White–Black, White–Latino, or White–Person of
Color interactions. Interethnic racial microaggressions oc-
cur between people of color as well. In the area of coun-
seling and therapy, for example, research may also prove
beneficial in understanding cross-racial dyads in which the
therapist is a person of color and the client is White or in
which both therapist and client are persons of color. Inves-
tigating these combinations of cross-racial dyads would be
useful, because it is clear that no racial/ethnic group is
immune from inheriting the racial biases of the society
(D. W. Sue, 2003). We encourage future research in these
two areas because all forms of microaggressions have
detrimental consequences.
REFERENCES
Abelson, R. P., Dasgupta, N., Park, J., & Banaji, M. R. (1998). Percep-
tions of the collective other. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
2, 243–250.
Advisory Board to the President’s Initiative on Race. (1998). One Amer-
ica in the 21st century: Forging a new future. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
American Counseling Association. (1999). Racism: Healing its effects.
Alexandria, VA: Author.
American Psychological Association. (2003). Guidelines on multicultural
education, training, research, practice, and organizational change for
psychologists. American Psychologist, 58, 377– 402.
Banaji, M. R., Hardin, C., & Rothman, A. J. (1993). Implicit stereotyping
in person judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,
272–281.
Blair, I. V., Judd, C. M., & Chapleau, K. M. (2004). The influence of
afrocentric facial features in criminal sentencing. Psychological Sci-
ence, 15, 674 679.
Brondolo, E., Kelly, K. P., Coakley, V., Gordon, T., Thompson, S., &
Levy, E. (2005). The Perceived Ethnic Discrimination Questionnaire:
Development and preliminary validation of a community version. Jour-
nal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 335–365.
Burkard, A. W., & Knox, S. (2004). Effect of therapist color-blindness on
empathy and attributions in cross-cultural counseling. Journal of Coun-
seling Psychology, 51, 387–397.
Cardemil, E. V., & Battle, C. L. (2003). Guess who’s coming to therapy?
Getting comfortable with conversations about race and ethnicity in
psychotherapy. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34,
278 –286.
Chakraborty, A., & McKenzie, K. (2002). Does racial discrimination
cause mental illness? British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 475– 477.
Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D. R. (1999).
284 May–June 2007
American Psychologist
Racism as a stressor for African Americans. American Psychologist, 54,
805– 816.
Conyne, R. K., & Bemak, F. (2005). Journeys to professional excellence:
Lessons from leading counselor educators and practitioners. Alexan-
dria, VA: American Counseling Association.
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The
self-protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608
630.
Delgado-Romero, E. A., Rowland, M., & Galvan, N. (2005). The con-
tinuing and evolving challenge of race and ethnicity in empirical
counseling and counseling psychology research: A reply. Counseling
Psychologist, 33, 559 –564.
DeVos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). American White? Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 447–466.
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (1996). Affirmative action, unintentional
racial biases, and intergroup relations. Journal of Social Issues, 52,
51–75.
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2000). Aversive racism and selective
decisions: 1989 –1999. Psychological Science, 11, 315–319.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Kawakami, K., & Hodson, G. (2002). Why
can’t we all just get along? Interpersonal biases and interracial distrust.
Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8, 88 –102.
Franklin, A. J. (2004). From brotherhood to manhood: How Black men
rescue their relationships and dreams from the invisibility syndrome.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Gordon, J., & Johnson, M. (2003). Race, speech, and hostile educational
environment: What color is free speech? Journal of Social Philosophy,
34, 414 436.
Grencavage, L. M., & Norcross, J. C. (1990). Where are the commonal-
ities among the therapeutic common factors? Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 21, 372–378.
Helms, J. E. (1992). A race is a nice thing to have: A guide to being a
white person or understanding the white persons in your life. Topeka,
KS: Content Communications.
Helms, J. E. (1995). An update of Helms’s White and people of color
racial identity models. In J. G. Ponterotto, J. M. Casas, L. A. Suzuki, &
C. M. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural counseling (pp.
181–191). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Helms, J. E., & Cook, D. (1999). Using race and culture in counseling
and psychotherapy: Theory and process. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn
& Bacon.
Hinton, E. L. (2004, March/April). Microinequities: When small slights
lead to huge problems in the workplace. DiversityInc. (Available at
http://www.magazine.org/content/files/Microinequities.pdf)
Horvath, A. O., & Symonds, B. D. (1991). Relationship between working
alliance and outcome in psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 38, 139 –149.
Jones, J. M. (1997). Prejudice and racism (2nd ed.). Washington, DC:
McGraw-Hill.
Kearney, L. K., Draper, M., & Baron, A. (2005). Counseling utilization by
ethnic minority college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minor-
ity Psychology, 11, 272–285.
Keltner, D., & Robinson, R. J. (1996). Extremism, power, and imagined
basis of social conflict. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5,
101–105.
Klonoff, E. A., & Landrine, H. (1999). Cross-validation of the Schedule
of Racist Events. Journal of Black Psychology, 25, 231–254.
Knox, S., Burkard, A. W., Johnson, A. J., Suzuki, L. A., & Ponterotto,
J. G. (2003). African American and European American therapists’
experiences of addressing race in cross-racial psychotherapy dyads.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, 466 481.
Liang, C. T. H., Li, L. C., & Kim, B. S. K. (2004). The Asian American
Racism-Related Stress Inventory: Development, factor analysis, reli-
ability, and validity. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 103–114.
Locke, D. C., & Kiselica, M. S. (1999). Pedagogy of possibilities: Teach-
ing about racism in multicultural counseling courses. Journal of Coun-
seling and Development, 77, 80 86.
Lui, W. M., & Pope-Davis, D. B. (2005). The working alliance, therapy
ruptures and impasses, and counseling competence: Implications for
counselor training and education. In R. T. Carter (Ed.), Handbook of
racial-cultural psychology and counseling (pp. 148 –167). Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.
Martin, D. J., Garske, J. P., & Davis, M. K. (2000). Relations of the
therapeutic alliance with outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic
review. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 66, 832– 837.
McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern
Racism Scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice,
discrimination and racism (pp. 91–126). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
National Conference of Christians and Jews. (1992). Taking America’s
pulse: A summary report of the National Conference Survey on Inter-
Group Relations. New York: Author. (Available at http://eric.ed.gov/
ERICDocs/data/ericdocs2/content_storage_01/0000000b/80/23/84/59.pdf)
Nelson, T. D. (2006). The psychology of prejudice. Boston: Pearson.
Neville, H. A., Lilly, R. L., Duran, G., Lee, R., & Browne, L. (2000).
Construction and initial validation of the Color Blind Racial Attitudes
Scale (COBRAS). Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, 59 –70.
Pierce, C. (1995). Stress analogs of racism and sexism: Terrorism, torture,
and disaster. In C. Willie, P. Rieker, B. Kramer, & B. Brown (Eds.),
Mental health, racism, and sexism (pp. 277–293). Pittsburgh, PA:
University of Pittsburgh Press.
Pierce, C., Carew, J., Pierce-Gonzalez, D., & Willis, D. (1978). An
experiment in racism: TV commercials. In C. Pierce (Ed.), Television
and education (pp. 62– 88). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Plant, E. A., & Peruche, B. M. (2005). The consequences of race for police
officers’ responses to criminal suspects. Psychological Science, 16,
180 –183.
Ponterotto, J. G., Casas, J. M., Suzuki, L. A., & Alexander, C. M. (2001).
Handbook of multicultural counseling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
President’s Commission on Mental Health. (1978). Report of the Presi-
dent’s Commission on Mental Health. Washington, DC: U.S. Govern-
ment Printing Office.
Reid, L. D., & Radhakrishnan, P. (2003). Race matters: The relations
between race and general campus climate. Cultural Diversity and
Ethnic Minority Psychology, 9, 263–275.
Richardson, T. Q., & Molinaro, K. L. (1996). White counselor self-
awareness: A prerequisite for multicultural competence. Journal of
Counseling & Development, 74, 238 –242.
Ridley, C. R. (2005). Overcoming unintentional racism in counseling and
therapy (2nd ed.). Thousand, Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sanchez-Hucles, J., & Jones, N. (2005). Breaking the silence around race
in training, practice, and research. Counseling Psychologist, 33, 547–
558.
Sears, D. O. (1988). Symbolic racism. In P. A. Katz & D. A. Taylor
(Eds.), Eliminating racism: Profiles in controversy (pp. 53– 84). New
York: Plenum.
Sellers, R. M., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). The role of racial identity in
perceived racial discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 84, 1070 –1092.
Smedley, A., & Smedley, B. D. (2005). Race as biology is fiction, racism
as a social problem is real. American Psychologist, 60, 16 –26.
Solo´rzano, D., Ceja, M., & Yosso, T. (2000, Winter). Critical race theory,
racial microaggressions, and campus racial climate: The experiences of
African American college students. Journal of Negro Education, 69,
60 –73.
Spanierman, L. B., Armstrong, P. I., Poteat, V. P., & Beer, A. M. (2006).
Psychosocial Costs of Racism to Whites: Exploring patterns through
cluster analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 434 441.
Spanierman, L. B., & Heppner, M. J. (2004). Psychosocial Costs of
Racism to Whites Scale (PCRW): Construction and initial validation.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 249–262.
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group
image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. In M.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp.
379 440). New York: Academic Press.
Strong, S. R., & Schmidt, L. D. (1970). Expertness and influence in
counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 17, 81– 87.
Sue, D. W. (2003). Overcoming our racism: The journey to liberation.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Sue, D. W. (2004). Whiteness and ethnocentric monoculturalism: Making
the “invisible” visible. American Psychologist, 59, 759 –769.
Sue, D. W. (2005). Racism and the conspiracy of silence. Counseling
Psychologist, 33, 100 –114.
Sue, D. W., Arredondo, P., & McDavis, R. J. (1992). Multicultural
285May–June 2007
American Psychologist
competencies/standards: A call to the profession. Journal of Counseling
& Development, 70, 477– 486.
Sue, D. W., Bucceri, J., Lin, A. I., Nadal, K. L., & Torino, G. C. (2007).
Racial microaggressions and the Asian American experience. Cultural
Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13, 72– 81.
Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2003). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory
and practice (4th ed.). New York: Wiley.
Sue, S., Fujino, D. C., Hu, L., & Takeuchi, D. (1991). Community mental
health services for ethnic minority groups: A test of the cultural re-
sponsiveness hypothesis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 59, 533–540.
Thompson, C. E., & Neville, H. A. (1999). Racism, mental health, and
mental health practice. Counseling Psychologist, 27, 155–223.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2001). Mental health:
Culture, race, and ethnicity—A supplement to Mental Health: A Report
of the Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Admin-
istration, Center for Mental Health Services.
Utsey, S. O., Gernat, C. A., & Hammar, L. (2005). Examining White
counselor trainees’ reactions to racial issues in counseling and super-
vision dyads. Counseling Psychologist, 33, 449 478.
Utsey, S. O., & Ponterotto, J. G. (1996). Development and validation of
the Index of Race-Related Stress (IRRS). Journal of Counseling Psy-
chology, 43, 490 –502.
Vanman, E. J., Saltz, J. L., Nathan, L. R., & Warren, J. A. (2004). Racial
discrimination by low-prejudiced Whites. Psychological Science, 15,
711–719.
Williams, D. R., & Collins, C. (1995). US socioeconomic and racial
differences in health: Patterns and explanations. Annual Review of
Sociology, 21, 349 –386.
Williams, D. R., Lavizzo-Mourey, R., & Warren, R. C. (1994). The
concept of race and health status in America. Public Health Reports,
109, 26 41.
Williams, D. R., Neighbors, H. W., & Jackson, J. S. (2003). Racial/ethnic
discrimination and health: Findings from community studies. American
Journal of Public Health, 93, 200–208.
Woodhouse, S. S., Schlosser, L. Z., Crook, R. E., Ligiero, D. P., & Gelso,
C. J. (2003). Client attachment to therapist: Relations to transference
and client recollections of parental caregiving. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 50, 395– 408.
Young, G., & Davis-Russell, E. (2002). The vicissitudes of cultural
competence: Dealing with difficult classroom dialogue. In E. Davis-
Russell (Ed.), The California School of Professional Psychology hand-
book of multicultural education, research, intervention, and training
(pp. 37–53). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
MAIL TO:
Name
Address
City State Zip
APA Member #
AMPA17
Check enclosed (make payable to APA)
Charge my: VISA MasterCard American
Express
Cardholder Name
etaD .pxE.oN draC
Signature (Required for Charge)
BILLING ADDRESS:
Street
City State Zip
Daytime Phone
E-mail
ORDER FORM
Start my 2007 subscription to American Psychologist!
ISSN: 0003-066X
___ $249.00,
INDIVIDUAL NONMEMBER ___________
___ $649.00,
INSTITUTION ___________
In DC add 5.75% / In MD add 5% sales tax _______
TOTAL AMOUNT ENCLOSED $ _______
Subscription orders must be prepaid. (Subscriptions are on
a calendar year basis only.) Allow 4-6 weeks for delivery of
the first issue. Call for international subscription rates.
SEND THIS ORDER FORM TO:
American Psychological Association
Subscriptions
750 First Street, NE
Washington, DC 20002-4242
Or call 800-374-2721, fax 202-336-5568.
TDD/TTY 202-336-6123.
For subscription information, e-mail:
286 May–June 2007
American Psychologist